Forming New Orthographic Representations in Alzheimer’s Disease and Mild Cognitive Impairment

  1. María del Carmen Pérez-Sánchez 1
  2. María González-Nosti 1
  3. Elena Herrera 1
  1. 1 Department of Psychology, University of Oviedo, Oviedo, Spain
Revista:
Health, Aging & End of Life (HAEL): Revista Internacional de Investigación

ISSN: 2462-5175

Año de publicación: 2022

Volumen: 7

Número: 1

Páginas: 5-28

Tipo: Artículo

DOI: 10.17811/HAEL.7.1.2022.5-28 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openAcceso abierto editor

Otras publicaciones en: Health, Aging & End of Life (HAEL): Revista Internacional de Investigación

Objetivos de desarrollo sostenible

Resumen

When we read the same word several times, we end up forming an orthographicrepresentation of it that allows us to read it in a fluid way. Several investigationswere aimed at how healthy people, children or dyslexic adults learn new words,but little is known on how this process works on patients with neurodegenerativediseases, for example Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and mild cognitive impairment(MCI). Hence, the aim of this project was to analyze the formation of neworthographic representationsin these people. For that purpose, eight words of avery low frequency were selected, half of them short and half of them long, andwere presented six times to the participants, 4 of them in a narrative context. Thelearning process was measured accordingto the elimination of the length effectthat indicates the crossing from a sublexical reading to a lexical one. The resultsshowed the disappearance of the length effect in the control group, as well as a reduction in RTs in MCI participants and AD patients, without a disappearanceof length effect in these groups. All this indicates that learning was consolidatedin the control group, whereas in the MCI and the AD group it cannot be said thata new representation has formed

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Arango Lasprilla, J. C., Iglesias, J., & Lopera, F. (2003). Neuropsychological study of familial Alzheimer's disease caused by mutation E280A in the presenilin 1 gene. American Journal of ALzheimer's Disease & Other Dementias, 18(3), 137-146. doi:10.1177/153331750301800306
  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204-256.
  • Cuetos, F., González-Nosti, M., & Martínez, C. (2005). The picture-naming task in the analysis of cognitive deterioration in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology Press 19(6), 545-557.
  • Cuetos, F., Martinez, T., Martinez, C., Izura, C., & Ellis, A. W. (2003). Lexical processing in Spanish patients with probable Alzheimer's disease. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(3), 549-561.
  • Cuetos-Vega, F., Menéndez-González, M., & Calatayud-Noguera, T. (2007). Descripción de un nuevo test para la detección precoz de la enfermedad de Alzheimer. Revista de Neurología, 44(8), 469-474.
  • Cunningham, A. E. (2006). Accounting for children's orthographic learning while reading text: do children self-teach? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95(1), 56-77. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2006.03.008
  • Cunningham, A. E., Perry, K. E., Stanovich, K. E., & Share, D. L. (2002). Orthographic learning during reading: examining the role of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82(3), 185-199.
  • Dubois, B., & Albert, M. L. (2004). Amnestic MCI or prodromal Alzheimer's disease? The Lancet Neurology, 3(4), 246-248. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00710-0
  • Fahle, M., & Daum, I. (1997). Visual learning and memory as functions of age. Neuropsychologia, 35(12), 1583-1589.
  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: a windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35(1), 116-124.
  • Grady, C. L., McIntosh, A. R., Horwitz, B., Maisog, J. M., Ungerleider, L. G., Mentis, M. J., . . . Haxby, J. V. (1995). Age-related reductions in human recognition memory due to impaired encoding. Science, 269(5221), 218-221.
  • Greene, J. D., Baddeley, A. D., & Hodges, J. R. (1996). Analysis of the episodic memory deficit in early Alzheimer's disease: evidence from the doors and people test. Neuropsychologia, 34(6), 537-551.
  • Grönholm-Nyman, P., Rinne, J. O., & Laine, M. (2010). Learning and forgetting new names and objects in MCI and AD. Neuropsychologia, 48(4), 1079-1088. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.12.008
  • Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. (1995). Is semantic memory consistently impaired early in the course of Alzheimer's disease? Neuroanatomical and diagnostic implications. Neuropsychologia, 33(4), 441-459.
  • IBM Corp. Released 2012. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 21.0, Armonk, NY: IBM Corp
  • Kwok, R. K., & Ellis, A. W. (2014). Visual word learning in adults with dyslexia. Frontiers in Human Neurosciences, 8, 264. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00264
  • Kwok, R. K., & Ellis, A. W. (2015). Visual word learning in skilled readers of English. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Hove), 68(2), 326-349. doi:10.1080/17470218.2014.944549
  • Lobo, A., Saz, P., Marcos, G., Día, J. L., de la Cámara, C., Ventura, T., . . . Aznar, S. (1999). [Revalidation and standardization of the cognition mini-exam (first Spanish version of the Mini-Mental Status Examination) in the general geriatric population]. Medicina Clínica (Barc), 112(20), 767-774.
  • Lövdén, M., Wenger, E., Mårtensson, J., Lindenberger, U., & Bäckman, L. (2013). Structural brain plasticity in adult learning and development. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Review, 37(9 Pt B), 2296-2310. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.014
  • Maloney, E., Risko, E. F., O'Malley, S., & Besner, D. (2009). Tracking the transition from sublexical to lexical processing: on the creation of orthographic and phonological lexical representations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Hove), 62(5), 858-867. doi:10.1080/17470210802578385
  • Marinus, E., Nation, K., & de Jong, P. F. (2015). Density and length in the neighborhood: Explaining cross-linguistic differences in learning to read in English and Dutch. Journal Experimental Child Psychology, 139, 127-147. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.05.006
  • McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology, 34(7), 939-944.
  • Molinuevo, J. L., Valls-Pedret, C., & Rami, L. (2010). From mild cognitive impairment to prodromal Alzheimer disease: A nosological evolution. European Geriatric Medicine, 1(3), 146-154. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurger.2010.05.003
  • Ouellette, G. (2010). Orthographic learning in learning to spell: the roles of semantics and type of practice. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107(1), 50-58. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.009
  • Ouellette, G., & Fraser, J. R. (2009). What exactly is a yait anyway: the role of semantics in orthographic learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104(2), 239-251. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.05.001
  • Petersen, R. C. (2005). Mild cognitive impairment: useful or not? Alzheimer's & Dementia, 1(1), 5-10. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2005.06.013
  • Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., & Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Archives of Neurology, 56(3), 303-308.
  • Protopapas, A. (2007). CheckVocal: a program to facilitate checking the accuracy and response time of vocal responses from DMDX. Behavior Reserch Methods, 39(4), 859-862.
  • Schenk, S., Minda, J. P., Lech, R. K., & Suchan, B. (2016). Out of sight, out of mind: Categorization learning and normal aging. Neuropsychologia, 91, 222-233. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.013
  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151-218; discussion 219-126.
  • Share, D. L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72(2), 95-129. doi:10.1006/jecp.1998.2481
  • Sperling, R. A., Bates, J. F., Chua, E. F., Cocchiarella, A. J., Rentz, D. M., Rosen, B. R., . . . Albert, M. S. (2003). fMRI studies of associative encoding in young and elderly controls and mild Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 74(1), 44-50.
  • Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2017). Semantic and phonological influences on visual word learning in a transparent language. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Hove), 70(4), 772-781. doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1164733
  • Suárez-Coalla, P., Ramos, S., Alvarez-Cañizo, M., & Cuetos, F. (2014). Orthographic learning in dyslexic Spanish children. Annals of Dyslexia, 64(2), 166-181. doi:10.1007/s11881-014-0092-5
  • Suárez-Coalla, P., Álvarez-Cañizo, M., & Cuetos, F. (2016). Orthographic learning in Spanish children. Journal of Research in Reading, 39(3), 292-311.
  • Wang, H. C., Castles, A., Nickels, L., & Nation, K. (2011). Context effects on orthographic learning of regular and irregular words. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(1), 39-57. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.11.005
  • Weekes, B. S. (1997). Differential Effects of Number of Letters on Word and Nonword Naming Latency. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 50(2), 439-456. doi:10.1080/713755710